1 Introduction
The first years of a child’s life are crucial for his/her development. Research in the neuroscience field provides evidence that, during pregnancy, the child’s cerebral architecture is already being constructed and will form the basis for the acquisition of motor, cognitive and social skills. Among the innumerable factors that contribute to adequate child development it is possible to cite aspects like nutrition, care regarding hygiene, carer affection, diversity of motor stimuli and in the language field (PAUS, 2013)1. The family is the baby’s first socializing environment, and this is slowly expanded to other spaces, such as crèches and pre-school.
Besides this, various studies conducted in the international context indicate that attending pre-school is an effective measure to guarantee greater equality in educational opportunities, that is, it contributes to learning (present and future) and to a broader school trajectory, especially in the case of children from low socio-economic backgrounds. (PEISNER-FEINBERG et al., 2001; SAMMONS, 2006; SYLVA et al., 2010). However, in many countries, there is a gap in enrolment and/or in the quality of the pre-school program attended between children of advantaged and disadvantaged background (UNICEF, 2018). Moreover, early differences in cognitive development, before entering school, have been observed according to children socio-economic background, race/ethnicity, family structure, home environment, among others (BURKAM; LEE, 2002; BIERDINGER, 2010)2. Burkham and Lee (2002) argue that if disadvantage or at-risk children are systematically placed in lower-quality preschool programs, this might increase the already existing inequalities on children’s cognitive abilities and, thus, prevent attendance to preschool to enhance equality in educational opportunities.
In Brazil, there are still few studies about the impact of child education on children learning and school trajectory. For example, a study by Damiani et al. (2011), observed that students who attend pre-school tend to have longer school trajectories, and Campos et al. (2011a) observed that children who had attended high quality institutions presented better performance in the 2nd year of primary education than children who had not attended pre-school or had studied in institutions with unsatisfactory levels of quality. Nevertheless, the scarce studies undertaken in the Brazilian context are not longitudinal, and, therefore, present diverse limits on identification of causality between the quality of pre-school and learning, or pupils’ future trajectory. For example, we do not have studies with a baseline that informs what children know at the outset of compulsory schooling and that indicates the factors associated to differences at this initial moment of schooling and/or development and learning during the first school years.
In this context, this article has the aim of presenting the design of a longitudinal study that begins at pre-school and presents a representative probabilistic sample of the Rio de Janeiro municipal public system. It discusses the importance of research featuring robust design with adoption of systematic information, collected with rigor and precision, to act as a basis for education policy decision-making (DAGENAIS et al., 2012; MILLER; PASLEY, 2012), and argues that the design of the study presents potential for investigation of which pre-school characteristics (school inputs, organization of supply and pedagogic processes) can influence the school trajectory, and, in particular, literacy and numeracy in the Brazilian context3. The article also presents the results of analyses of data collected during the first wave of a longitudinal study, carried out at the entry to the first year of pre-school in the Rio de Janeiro municipal system, focusing on analysis to identify factors associated to child development at the start of pre-school, which has become the initial step of the compulsory schooling in Brazil4.
2 The longitudinal study and the baseline measurement
The longitudinal study performed in Rio de Janeiro city’s municipal public system is scheduled to have a duration of three years, 2017–2019. The children’s cognitive development is measured with the instrument developed by the Centre for Evaluation and Monitoring of Durham University, England, called Performance Indicator for Primary Schools (PIPS), currently used in 9 countries (THE UNIVERSITY OF WESTERN AUSTRALIA, 1999). Furthermore, the study presents another two measurements regarding child development: a) final motor skills – with a “drawing along a trail” test; and b) anaerobic physical fitness – Sitting and Raising test (ARAÚJO, 1999). The main objective is to identify pre-school characteristics (school inputs, organization of provision, teachers’ training, school practices), as well as pedagogic processes and patterns of classroom interactions associated to children’s cognitive development.
There are four main innovations in the study. The first is the understanding that it is not only important but necessary to obtain direct measurements about child development. International studies have already demonstrated that ensuring access or measuring the quality of the environment are insufficient metrics to guarantee effective learning for children and youths in general (PEISNER-FEINBERG et al., 2000; SYLVA et al., 2010). Therefore, measuring child development is a necessary step in assessing the effectiveness of early childhood education policy.
The second innovation is the use of an adaptive cognitive test as a robust measurement at the individual level. Our preliminary analyses of the cognitive test results in Waves 1 and 2 of the project suggested a correlation of about 0.75 – higher than that observed in similar studies in Brazil, and very close to that found in international studies (SIRIN, 2005; TYMMS; MERRELL, JONES, 2004). The adaptive test allows generation of reliable measurements at the individual level without exposing the child to many items and reduced time for application.
The third refers to longitudinal data collection that offers consecutive measurements of the same children. This type of design is far superior to analyses with cross-section data and greatly improves the quality of the causal inference about the effects of the educational policies and teaching practices on child development. The majority of the studies that attempt to observe the impact of quality service in early childhood education or of the characteristics of schools on child development and/or other school results are correlational studies, and, therefore, always present limits or threats that a selection bias and/or variables omitted from the estimated models could explain the relations observed (GOLDSTEIN, 1997; NICHD EARLY CHILD CARE RESEARCH NETWORK; DUNCAN, 2003).
The fourth innovation is the possibility of comparing the child development in the Rio do Janeiro public system with those beginning the formal schooling process in other countries participating in the Placing Early Childhood Education at the Heart of Worldwide Policy Making (iPIPS). The data collected will allow comparison of the development level of Rio de Janeiro pupils with others of the same age in different educational systems at the formal starting point, and, more important, how they develop in the first school years.
Thus, the project arouse from the need to construct more robust research designs that allow better quality causal inference to understand the specific effects of existing education policies in Brazil’s public systems.
3 Cognitive instrument of the PIPS
The instrument is composed of a set of dimensions of cognitive assessment measurements, such as: a) Handwriting – the child is requested to write his own name; b) Vocabulary – identifying objects in a series of images; c) Ideas about reading – to assess concepts about print; c) Phonological awareness – rhymes and word repetition; d) Identification of letters; e) Recognition of words and reading – sentences and comprehension; f) Ideas about mathematics – understanding of mathematical concepts; g) Counting and numbers; h) Addition and subtraction problems without symbols; i) Shapes identification; j) Digit Identification; and k) Mathematical problems including sums with symbols.
The application of the test is individual and it may be conducted by the group’s own teacher or another adult, and the duration varies from 10 to 20 minutes. In the longitudinal study, a researcher asks the questions and makes a record of the answers on a tablet. The program is adjusted such that wrong and right answers are considered in order to continue the test or not. Each test session presents items of increasing difficulty, which enables testing with a desirable minimum duration, without making the child bored with questions that are too simple or getting upset when faced with very difficult ones (TYMMS; MERREL; JONES, 2004).
Currently, the iPIPS is being applied to children in the age range 4–7, with adapted instruments, in 9 countries, there having been various scientific works generated that attest to its technical quality and predictive power. The English version of PIPS presented a high test-retest reliability, a correlation of 0.98 (THE UNIVERSITY OF WESTERN AUSTRALIA, 1999) and good predictive power for reading and mathematics. Using a baseline measurement with children aged 4 to 5, the test presents a correlation of 0.71 for reading 3 years after the first measurement and a correlation of 0.7 after 7 years (TYMMS, 1999; TYMMS; MERREL; HENDERSON, 1997; TYMMS; MERREKK; JONES, 2004; TYMMS; MERREL; JONES, 2004). These are high values compared with those of other tests available, which reinforces its relevance for use in school effectiveness studies with a longitudinal or experimental design.
In Brazil, 4 important steps of the longitudinal study have already been taken: (i) adaptation of the test (items) to Portuguese; (ii) cultural adaptation of the figures presented in the test (booklet); (iii) production of the software that helps researchers with application of the test and (iv) pre-testing of the items and (v) data collection for three waves of the longitudinal study. The pre-test was conducted in 2016 in three cities, with 560 children. The analyses of the pre-test indicated that the cognitive test items presented appropriate behavior, suggesting theoretical adequacy of the items and it revealed the adequacy of the scale for children aged 4–7 in the Brazilian context5.
4 Study design and sample
The project, started in March 2017, presents a probabilistic single-stage cluster sample (school as the primary sampling unit) stratified by characteristics of pre-school provision6 and local authority of the municipal system of the city of Rio de Janeiro. It ran a lottery to the random selection of 46 schools from the Rio de Janeiro Municipal System7. All the children enrolled in the first-year of pre-school of the selected/sampled schools were eligible to participate in the research.
In order to monitor the students in the longitudinal study, we adopted the following strategy: a) all the students enrolled in the schools randomly selected for the sample would be measured in all the waves, irrespective of whether they had moved to another school of the Rio de Janeiro public system. Children who had migrated to private schools or to other cities ceased to be part of the research; b) students that had joined the randomly selected schools in the sample after starting the study were also tested.
Figure 1 presents the different stages in the data collection. It is to be stressed that the article analyzes only the data collected in Wave 1 – entry to pre-school.
Some challenges were identified right at the beginning of the longitudinal study, among which was the difficulty in accessing some districts due to violence, thus causing an impact on the research fieldwork. The risk to the physical integrity of the researchers involved in the study was the sole criterion adopted to substitute the randomly selected schools. Contact was made with all 11 Regional Education Coordinating Authorities (CREs), and the school boards to map the specific cases of violence in different city regions. Seven schools were replaced by others (random sampling with replacement). However, in one CRE, extreme difficulty was faced in selecting schools in risk-free areas, and, therefore, a decision was taken to intentionally select a school. Future analyses will consider this aspect of the sample, including and excluding a school selected for convenience to identify possible alterations in the results.
Chart 1 presents the descriptive statistic of the data collected in 2017, the first year of research.
Sample, school mobility and attrition | Number of cases | Percent |
---|---|---|
Children assessed in wave 1 | 2,716 | 100.0% |
Children assessed in wave 2 | 2,848 | - |
Children not found in Rio de Janeiro´s system (other systems) | 106 | 3.9% |
Children that moved to other schools within Rio de Janeiro´s public system | 164 | 6.0% |
Children still enrolled in schools of Rio de Janeiro´s public system in October 2017 | 2,610 | 96.1% |
Children still in the schools of the sample in October 2017 | 2,446 | 90.1% |
Children that moved to 2nd year or preschool classrooms | 56 | 2.1% |
Total children with two measures | 2,237 | 82.4% |
Source: Authors own elaboration, 2017.
5 What do children know upon entry to preschool?
Figure 2 shows the item-person map for the mathematics test collected in the first wave. The map allows analysis of four principal points: a) distribution of the children on the test scale; b) distribution of the items on the scale; c) level of difficulty of the items; d) probability of each child correctly identifying the different items. It is possible to observe that the distribution of tested children is very close to a normal distribution, that is, few children at the lower and upper ends of the scale, and a greater concentration in the intermediate part. The results suggest that the test is capable of adequately measuring the mathematical knowledge at the moment of school entry in the Rio de Janeiro municipal public system.
Another relevant aspect is to analyze the distribution of items on the scale. Items in the lower part were estimated to be the easiest, whereas those positioned in the upper part were the most difficult. It is important to stress that, as the test applied is adaptive, most of the students had the application interrupted at some moment of the test when they demonstrated they did not know certain content. For instance, children incapable of identifying single-digit numbers were not exposed to the more difficult numbers session – with two or three digits.
The scale for mathematics covered a broad array of skills. The order of the items is relevant for testing the hypothesis that there is a progression in the level of difficulty of the items. We can observe that the easier items are those in the initial test sections – Ideas about mathematics – and afterwards there appeared items about counting objects, identification of shapes and single-digit numbers. Thus, children at the beginning of the scale dominate concepts like size (bigger/smaller), quantity and volume (more/less). They are capable, for example, of pointing to the biggest cat in a drawing with cats of three different sizes; pointing to which of two people holding balloons in a drawing has more. Children in the sample close to the average typically, besides the concepts described above, are also capable of identifying single-digit numbers (1–5) and counting a small quantity of objects with the support of images. Other students start pre-school already being capable of identifying double-digit numbers and have notions of simple formal arithmetic (for instance, doing simple sums) and a very small group with more advanced knowledge are capable of identifying triple-digit numbers and performing formal mathematical operations with coins. The broad scale of the test is necessary to adequately measure, in the same metric, students at various stages of development from the first year of pre-school to the end of the first year of primary education.
Another way of presenting the results of the item-person map is to make a pedagogic interpretation of the scale. Figure 3 presents the percentages of students at each development stage for mathematics at the entry to pre-school in March 2017.
At admission to pre-school, most pupils (79.6%) were at the initial levels on the scale, still developing skills related to ideas about mathematics, counting objects and identifying single-digit numbers (especially 1 – 5). Only 18.8% of the pupils were already capable of identifying numbers 6 – 10, and making simple informal sums, and a very small portion (1.6%) identified double-digit numbers and were capable of making more complex informal sums or simple formal sums.
The data presented are useful for teachers, principals, parents and researchers in the education field and administrators. Identifying what students are capable of achieving at entry to pre-school can help with school planning and debates about the pre-school curriculum. As it constitutes a probabilistic sample for the Rio de Janeiro municipal public network,
It is important to emphasize that the ladder does not indicate what students should be capable of doing at the end of a certain stage. The figures only present the starting point and the pace of development of children in different dimensions. The pedagogic interpretation of the cognitive test results is a fundamental task for maximizing the dialogue and comprehension of the results with school actors.
The item-person map was also constructed for the language part of the test and the pedagogic ladder was made considering the items of ideas about reading, identification of letters and reading of words and sentences. Figure 4 presents the pedagogic interpretation of the language measurement, considering the aforementioned sections.
Approximately two thirds of the children enter pre-school knowing only the first letter of their own names and with basic ideas about what reading and writing are (for example, the item asks the child to point to “someone who is reading” and “someone who is writing”). The other third already know the majority of the letters and are capable of identifying on a page that presents a drawing and a text “where the sentence begins”. Very few students (0.5%) enter pre-school reading words and simple sentences.
6 How do the skills vary according to the characteristics of children and their families?
As observed in the previous section, the children already present a great variation regarding the development of language and mathematical skills right at the beginning of pre-school. The next analysis will describe how this variation is associated with the children’s characteristics and the family environment, as well as the attendance of a crèche. For such, we use two sources: i) data from questionnaires for children’s parents/guardians (parents’ schooling, access to cash transfer programs, home learning environment and possession of goods; ii) data from the Sistema de Gestão Acadêmica – SGA (Academic Administration System) of the Rio de Janeiro Municipal Education Secretariat: gender, age, parents’ schooling, skin color, access to income transfer programs, and frequency to crèche.
Neither of the two databases contain complete information for all the children assessed and as it has only been possible, until the moment, to apply the questionnaire to 62.0% of the parents/guardians, we have used data available from the SGA to complete part of the missing data referring to parents’ schooling, access to cash transfer programs, and if the child has attended a crèche. Chart 2 presents the description of the variables utilized and their sources.
Name | Type | Description | Source |
---|---|---|---|
Dependent Variables | |||
Language | Continuous | Language measurement Wave 1 | Longitudinal Study (LS) |
Mathematics | Continuous | Mathematics measurement Wave 1 | Longitudinal Study (LS) |
Explicit Variables – Child | |||
Gender | Dummy | Indicates child´s gender (0 = girl; 1 = boy) | SGA/SME |
Age | Discrete | Indicates child’s age in months | SGA/SME |
Primary education complete/incomplete | Dummy | Indicates whether at least one of the child’s parents/guardian(s) has completed primary education (0 = no; 1 = yes) | SGA/SME and LS |
High school | Dummy | Indicates whether at least one parent/guardian has completed high school (0 = no; 1 = yes) | SGA/SME and LBB |
Higher education | Dummy | Indicates whether at least one parent/guardian has completed Higher education (0 = no; 1 = yes) | SGA/SME and LS |
Non White | Dummy | Indicates the child’s skin color (declared by the parent[s]/guardian[s]) (0 = white; 1 = not white) | SGA/SME |
Poverty | Dummy | Indicates whether the child’s parent(s)/guardian(s) is/are benefited by cash transfer programs (0 = no; 1 = yes) | SGA/SME and LS |
Home learning environment | Continuous | Indicator elaborated using the IRT (Item Response Theory), regarding items referring to a learning process and the physical environment at home8 | Longitudinal Study (LS) |
Possession of goods | Continuous | Indicator elaborated using the IRT, regarding items referring to possession of goods and access to services9 | Longitudinal Study (LS) |
Crèche | Dummy | Indicates whether the child attended crèche (0 = no; 1 = yes) | SGA/SME |
Source: Authors own elaboration, 2017.
Longitudinal Study in Rio de Janeiro (LS).
Table 1 presents the mean and standard deviation of the mathematics and language measurements at pre-school entry (Wave 1), according to gender, age, skin color, parent’s (s’)/guardian’s(s’) educational level, and attending a crèche.
Mathematics | Language | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | |
Girls | -1,565 | 1,381 | -0,321 | 0,961 |
Boys | -1,597 | 1,341 | -0,508 | 1,048 |
Incomplete/complete elementary education | -1,871 | 1,279 | -0,592 | 0,944 |
High school | -1,405 | 0,286 | -0,286 | 1,004 |
Higher education | -0,711 | 0,003 | -0,003 | 1,104 |
White | -1,423 | 1,372 | -0,347 | 1,024 |
Non white | -1,681 | 1,338 | -0,455 | 1,004 |
Without cash transfer | -1,529 | 1,385 | -0,391 | 1,022 |
With cash transfer | -1,756 | 1,260 | -0,497 | 0,965 |
From 48 to 50 months | -2,173 | 1,333 | -0,843 | 1,018 |
From 51 to 53 months | -1,773 | 1,271 | -0,535 | 0,980 |
From 54 to 56 months | -1,472 | 1,253 | -0,324 | 0,951 |
57 months or more | -1,116 | 1,339 | -0,102 | 0,950 |
Has not attended crèche | -1,565 | 1,381 | -0,388 | 1,011 |
Has attended crèche | -1,600 | 1,336 | -0,448 | 1,008 |
Source: Authors own elaboration, 2018.
Regarding the child’s family characteristics, this table indicates a marked difference between the development level in language and mathematics, in accordance with the parents’ schooling, with a disadvantage for children whose parents had a maximum of complete or incomplete elementary education. The disadvantage regarding poverty and skin color seem to be less visible. Another marked difference is related to age, that is, younger children present starting points lower than those who are older8. This tendency shows the strong effect of maturity in this age range. Finally, girls present an advantage over boys with respect to the starting point in language, and children who have attended a crèche presented a slight disadvantage at their starting point in mathematics and language in comparison to those who have not9.
Table 2 presents the results obtained from linear regressions estimating the language and mathematics measurements at entry to pre-school, based on child and family characteristics, and whether the child attended a crèche or not. In Model 1 we inserted the contextual variables of the students who had little missing data (gender, age, parents’ education, poverty and crèche). In Model 2 we inserted the home learning environment and goods possession variables. The parameters estimated with the different models are reported as effect sizes, which were calculated in accordance with the methodology developed by Tymms (2004).
Mathematics | Language | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 1 | Model 2 | |
Gender | 0,005 | -0,053 | -0,172*** | -0,225*** |
Age | 0,665*** | 0,747*** | 0,618*** | 0,719*** |
High School | 0,385*** | 0,381*** | 0,339*** | 0,213*** |
Higher Education | 0,823*** | 0,711*** | 0,563*** | 0,334** |
Non White | -0,125** | -0,155** | -0,063 | -0,051 |
Poverty | -0,117* | -0,067 | -0,013 | 0,033 |
Home learning environment | - | 0,243*** | - | 0,275*** |
Possession of Goods | - | 0,130* | - | 0,281*** |
Crèche | -0,049 | -0,389 | -0,097* | -0,008 |
R2 | 0,155 | 0,198 | 0,126 | 0,176 |
N | 2022 | 1182 | 2022 | 1182 |
Source: Authors own elaboration, 2018.
* p < 0.05, ** p<0.01, *** p <0.001.
Note: Incomplete/complete elementary education is the reference category for parents’ education.
In Model 1, which estimated the mathematics measurement in Wave 1, we observed a strong association among the children’s development, age and parents’ education. We also detected a small disadvantage for non-white children and those in a situation of poverty. Controlling for other variables in the models, attending a crèche did not seem to be associated with the children’s starting point.
The lack or small negative impact of attending crèche has been observed in other studies (Campos et al., 2011ba; Loeb et al., 2004), which suggests that it shall be associated to the quality of provision10. In our sample, it is relevant to mention that low-income families have priority access to enrollment in public municipal crèches and, therefore, there is a selection bias that might explain the association observed.
In Model 2, we noticed that the home learning environment is a variable with a modest effect on both mathematics and language starting point, even when controlling for parents’ educational level. The variable, possession of goods presented a small but significant association with the wave 1 starting point in mathematics. With the insertion of these variables, poverty ceased to be significant, which is probably due to the fact these variables are capturing this dimension in a more adequate manner. Nevertheless, the results observed need to be considered with caution, as these indicators were devised based only on items of the questionnaire applied to pupils’ parent(s)/guardian(s), and their insertion into the model generated a great loss of observations from the sample. Even so, such indicators increased the explanatory capacity of the model, and seem promising for understanding other aspects of the variation in the family context that help to explain as much the variation in starting point as, possibly, the learning during pre-school. We expect that further analyses using longitudinal data for children’s progress present a larger R2for the regression models.
The results of the models that estimated the language measurement in Wave 1 present similar patterns to those in models that estimated the mathematics measurement in relation to the schooling of the parent(s)/guardian(s) and the child’s age. However, in Model 1, we observed that boys were at a disadvantage at the initial point in language, and the parameters estimated for skin color and poverty were not statistically significant. In this model, attending a crèche seemed to represent a disadvantage. In Model 2, the variables, home learning environment and possession of goods seem to be moderately associated to the starting point in language, and, with the insertion of these variables, the parameters estimated for attending a crèche ceased to be statistically significant.
The results found tend to be aligned with studies conducted in the Brazilian context that indicate that parents’ education, socio-economic level and/or poverty and gender are associated to students’ performance in the initial years of primary school (BONAMINO et al., 2010; FRANCO et al., 2007; SOARES, 2003; 2004). Nevertheless, these studies utilize data from assessment systems, the majority of which present cross-sectional designs11. Thus, they estimate the association of the students’ characteristics with their performance in the 3rd and/or 4th/5th grades of primary education, without having a control of the students’ previous performance. Even the studies that utilize longitudinal data, as is the case of studies that relied on data from “A Longitudinal study of quality and equity of Brazilian Elementary Education: GERES 2005”, or from the Rio de Janeiro municipality, counted on only on a baseline measure for the 1st or 2nd grade of primary education (BROOKE et al., 2014; SOARES et al., 2017) or at the end of the 1st grade (CASTRO, 2016) and/or the 3rd grade (BARTHOLO; COSTA, 2016). Thus, the study contributes to discussion and understanding of the effect of family and child characteristics right at the beginning of compulsory schooling, that is at the beginning of pre-school.
7 Final considerations
The data presented describe what the children enrolled in the Rio de Janeiro city municipal public system know and are capable of doing at the entry to pre-school, considering the cognitive data collected in the first wave of the longitudinal study. The simple description of the scale and its pedagogic interpretation may be useful information for administrators, principals and teachers to think about curriculum and pedagogical planning for the students who enter the aforementioned school system.
The adaptive test, with the use of the software and a tablet, allows application of a test with a broad scale with the least amount of time possible (BARTHOLO et al., 2019, forthcoming). The diversity of skills among the students upon the pre-school entry, Figure 2 (Items Map), is a reality that is little studied and debated in the educational field in the Brazilian context, and challenges all teachers who deal with this age range.
The multivariate regression models suggest that there are factors associated to the children’s level of development at the entry to the compulsory schooling process. The first characteristic that is, at times, ignored by the professionals and researchers in the educational field is age. Pupils born in January, February and March of year “X”, on average, present a lower stage of development compared with their peers born in the previous year, “X - 1”, in April, May and June. This information must be considered by teachers, head teachers and other professionals involved in school activities and learning, not only during pre-school, but also in the first years of primary school.
The educational level of parents/guardians is the most significant factor in the attempts to explain pupils’ initial development stage. The results corroborate international evidence about factors associated to the learning of children and youths (GORARD; SEE, 2013) and studies that focus on children starting school (BURKHAM; LEE, 2002; BIERDINGER, 2010). Furthermore, there is a slight disadvantage for children living in poverty or that are not white. The home learning environment indicator suggests a positive association with the cognitive test, even after control of socio-economic level and age variables. This evidence may be utilized by policy-makers for the formulation of action with parents and guardians to disseminate activities that can be stimulated in the children’s home to bolster schoolwork.
Last but not least, longitudinal studies are important to know children’s learning trajectory at school as much as a baseline children measure which allows noticing what children know at the outset of compulsory schooling. Knowing the differences at the initial moment of schooling associated with home background characteristics can help to understand child development at the beginning of pre-school and, thus, identify potential policies and practices that can reduce such inequalities during pre-school.